Rbt Task List Measurement Featured Image 2

RBT Task List – Measurement (A-02)

Welcome back to our in-depth exploration of Section A: Measurement on the RBT Task List!

Having thoroughly covered the critical first step, A-01: Prepare for Data Collection, we now move into the active implementation of some of the most fundamental measurement techniques used in Applied Behavior Analysis.

Let’s dive deep into:

A-02: Implementing Continuous Measurement Procedures

This task is all about capturing every instance of a target behavior during a defined observation period, providing a complete and detailed record.

Clear Explanation: What is Continuous Measurement?

Once all preparations are complete and you’re ready to begin, the RBT moves into the active implementation of measurement procedures.

Continuous measurement procedures are those that aim to capture and record every single instance of the target behavior during a defined observation period.

This is a key distinction from discontinuous measurement procedures (which we’ll cover in A-03, such as partial interval, whole interval, and momentary time sampling), where only a sample of behavior within specific, pre-set intervals is recorded.

Continuous measurement provides a complete and comprehensive record of the behavior’s occurrence, offering rich, detailed data for analysis by your supervising BCBA.

The RBT Task List highlights five key types of continuous measurement that RBTs must be proficient in: Frequency, Rate, Duration, Latency, and Interresponse Time (IRT).

Mastery of each, including their specific nuances and appropriate applications, is essential for effective RBT practice.

Why is Continuous Measurement Used?

These methods are often preferred when the target behaviors:

  • Have a clear, observable beginning and end (i.e., they are discrete).
  • Do not occur at such an extremely high rate that accurately recording every instance becomes practically impossible for a human observer.
  • Require a precise count or exact timing for effective analysis and intervention planning (e.g., tracking the total time spent engaging in a specific behavior, or how quickly a client responds to an instruction).

This task requires RBTs to not only understand the definition of each continuous measurement procedure but also to execute them accurately and consistently, often under dynamic real-world conditions.

It demands focused attention, precision in observation, and the correct use of appropriate measurement tools.

Let’s break down each type of continuous measurement in detail:

(A-02a) Frequency

  • Detailed Definition:
    Frequency, in its simplest terms, is a direct count of the number of times a specific behavior occurs during a defined observation period. It’s one of the most straightforward and commonly used measures in ABA. The result of a frequency count is always a whole number (e.g., 5 instances, 12 instances).
    For frequency to be a meaningful and interpretable measure, the length of the observation period should ideally be consistent across different observation sessions.
    If the observation periods vary, then the frequency count should always be reported alongside the length of the observation period.
    This allows for proper context and comparison over time. If observation periods vary significantly, converting the frequency count to a rate (which we’ll discuss next in A-02b) becomes more appropriate and informative.
    • Key Characteristics: Involves tallying discrete, countable instances of behavior. Each occurrence of the behavior (as per its operational definition) is counted.
    • When to Use Frequency: This method is best suited for behaviors that:
      • Have a clear onset (beginning) and offset (end).
      • Are relatively brief in duration.
      • Don’t occur so rapidly that accurately counting each instance becomes impractical or unreliable.
      • Examples: Number of times a student raises their hand in class, number of aggressive acts (e.g., hits, kicks) during a play session, number of mands (requests) made by a client during a therapy hour, number of times a client correctly identifies a picture card.
  • Real-World Examples of Frequency Recording:
    • Counting Correct Responses in DTT:
      • An RBT is conducting Discrete Trial Training (DTT) with a child who is learning to identify colors. The RBT presents a red card and asks, “What color is this?” The child says, “Red.”
        The RBT clicks a handheld tally counter once. Over a set of 20 trials, the RBT records the frequency of correct responses.
      • Observation Period: 20 discrete trials.
      • Frequency Data Example: 18 correct responses.
    • Tracking Instances of Self-Stimulatory Behavior (Stereotypy):
      • An RBT is observing a client during a 30-minute free play period.
        The target behavior is hand-flapping, operationally defined as “any instance of the client’s hands moving rapidly up and down at the wrist or elbow for at least 3 consecutive seconds, with at least 5 seconds of no hand-flapping separating episodes.”
        Each distinct episode of hand-flapping that meets this definition is counted.
      • Observation Period: 30 minutes.
      • Frequency Data Example: 7 episodes of hand-flapping.
    • Monitoring Social Initiations During a Community Outing:
      • An RBT is supporting a teenager in a community outing (e.g., a visit to a local youth club). One of the goals is to increase appropriate social initiations towards peers.
        The RBT counts each time the teenager independently (without prompting) says “Hi” to a peer, asks a peer a relevant question, or offers to share an item or join an activity.
      • Observation Period: 1-hour community outing.
      • Frequency Data Example: 4 social initiations.
  • Key Terms/Vocabulary (Specific to Frequency):
    • Count: Synonymous with frequency; the total number of occurrences.
    • Tally: A mark (e.g., a checkmark, a line) made to keep score or count occurrences of a behavior. Often done on a data sheet.
    • Event Recording: Another term frequently used to describe frequency recording, as you are recording each “event” or discrete occurrence of the behavior.
    • Observation Period: The defined timeframe during which the behavior is observed and counted. This is crucial for context: A frequency of 10 instances of behavior in a 5-minute period is vastly different from a frequency of 10 instances in a 5-hour period.
  • Common Mistakes/Misunderstandings (Specific to Frequency Recording):
    • Inconsistent Observation Periods Without Context: Counting behavior for 10 minutes one day and 30 minutes the next, then directly comparing the raw frequency counts without acknowledging the difference in observation time. This is where converting to rate becomes more useful.
    • Unclear Operational Definition of “One Instance”: If the operational definition isn’t precise enough, it can be difficult to determine when one instance of the behavior ends and another begins.
      This can lead to over-counting or under-counting.
      For example, for “crying,” is each individual sob counted as a new instance, or is a continuous bout of crying considered one instance until there’s a pause of a certain length? The operational definition must clarify this.
    • Observer Reactivity: The RBT’s presence or obvious data collection methods (e.g., very conspicuously clicking a tally counter every time the behavior occurs) might influence the client’s behavior, leading to an inaccurate (either inflated or suppressed) frequency count.
      RBTs should strive to be as unobtrusive as possible.
    • Attempting to Count Very High-Rate Behaviors: If a behavior occurs extremely rapidly (e.g., some vocal tics that might happen multiple times per second, or rapid finger-flicking), accurately counting each instance using simple frequency recording can become impossible and unreliable.
      Other measurement systems (like interval recording or time sampling) might be more appropriate for such behaviors.
    • Forgetting to Reset Tally Counters: If using a manual tally counter, forgetting to reset it to zero before a new observation period or session begins will lead to cumulative, inaccurate counts.

(A-02b) Rate

  • Detailed Definition:
    Rate of responding is a measure of frequency (the count of behavior) per unit of time. It is calculated by dividing the total frequency of the behavior by the total duration of the observation period.
    Rate is a particularly useful and often more informative measure than raw frequency because it allows for direct and standardized comparison of behavior across observation periods that may vary in length.
    For example, if a client mands (requests items) 5 times in a 10-minute observation period, their rate of manding is 0.5 mands per minute.
    If, on another day, they mand 15 times in a 30-minute observation period, their rate of manding is still 0.5 mands per minute (15 mands / 30 minutes).
    Even though the raw frequency is different (5 vs. 15), the rate of manding is the same, indicating a consistent level of this skill when time is factored in.
    This provides a more standardized and comparable measure of behavior.
    • Formula: Rate = Frequency / Time
    • Units: Rate is always expressed as a count per unit of time (e.g., responses per minute, occurrences per hour, kicks per session – if session length is standard).
    • When to Use Rate:
      • Essential when observation times vary across sessions or observation periods.
      • When you want to understand the fluency or proficiency of a behavior (how quickly and efficiently it can be performed).
      • For behaviors that can occur many times and for which simply knowing the count isn’t as informative as knowing how often it happens within a specific timeframe.
  • Real-World Examples of Rate Calculation:
    • Academic Fluency (e.g., Math Facts):
      • An RBT is working with a student on improving their speed and accuracy with multiplication facts. The student correctly answers 40 multiplication problems during a 2-minute timed probe.
      • Frequency = 40 correct problems.
      • Time = 2 minutes.
      • Rate = 40 problems / 2 minutes = 20 correct problems per minute.
    • Aggression Across Different Session Lengths:
      • On Monday, a client exhibited 6 instances of hitting during a 60-minute therapy session.
      • Rate = 6 hits / 60 minutes = 0.1 hits per minute (or, if preferred, 6 hits per hour).
      • On Tuesday, the therapy session was unexpectedly cut short to only 45 minutes due to a fire drill, and the client exhibited 3 instances of hitting during that time.
      • Frequency = 3 hits.
      • Rate = 3 hits / 45 minutes = approximately 0.067 hits per minute (or, if converted to per hour, 4 hits per hour).
      • Comparing the rates (0.1 hits/min vs. 0.067 hits/min) provides a more accurate picture of potential change or stability in the behavior than just comparing the raw frequencies (6 hits vs. 3 hits), because the observation time was different.
    • Vocal Stereotypy During Free Play:
      • An RBT observes a client for a 15-minute segment during free play and counts 30 instances of a specific repetitive vocalization (as per the operational definition).
      • Frequency = 30 vocalizations.
      • Time = 15 minutes (or 0.25 hours).
      • Rate = 30 vocalizations / 15 minutes = 2 vocalizations per minute (or, if the BCBA prefers per hour, 120 vocalizations per hour).
  • Key Terms/Vocabulary (Specific to Rate):
    • Per Unit of Time: The defining characteristic that distinguishes rate from simple frequency (e.g., per minute, per hour, per session if session length is consistently standard).
    • Celeration: While RBTs do not typically calculate celeration themselves, it’s a related concept. Celeration refers to the change in rate over time (i.e., acceleration if the rate is increasing, or deceleration if the rate is decreasing).
      Understanding rate is foundational to understanding celeration charts (like the Standard Celeration Chart) used by BCBAs for more advanced analysis of behavior change.
  • Common Mistakes/Misunderstandings (Specific to Rate Calculation & Reporting):
    • Using Inconsistent Time Units for Comparison: Calculating rate per minute one day and rate per hour the next day, and then trying to compare these values directly without converting them to a common unit.
      Always standardize the time unit when making comparisons across different data points.
    • Calculation Errors: Simple arithmetic mistakes when dividing the frequency by the time. It’s always good practice to double-check calculations, especially if using a calculator.
    • Reporting Rate for Very Low Frequency Behaviors: If a behavior happens only once during a very long observation period (e.g., once in a 3-hour observation), reporting it as “0.33 occurrences per hour” might be mathematically correct but less practically meaningful or intuitive than simply stating.
      “The behavior occurred once during the 3-hour observation period.”
      The supervising BCBA will guide when rate is the most informative way to present the data.
    • Not Recording the Observation Time Accurately and Precisely: If the “time” component of the rate calculation is incorrect (e.g., the RBT estimates the observation period instead of timing it precisely), the resulting rate will be skewed and inaccurate.
    • Confusing “Session” as a Standard Time Unit if Sessions Vary: If therapy sessions vary in length from day to day, reporting behavior as “responses per session” is not a true rate unless all sessions are of identical duration.
      It’s generally more standard and informative to report rates as “responses per minute” or “responses per hour.”

(A-02c) Duration

  • Detailed Definition:
    Duration refers to the total amount of time a specific behavior occurs, measured from its clear onset (beginning) to its clear offset (end).
    It measures how long a single instance of a behavior lasts, or the cumulative total of time spent engaging in that behavior across multiple instances within an observation period.
    There are two main ways duration is often recorded, and the supervising BCBA will specify which is needed:
    • Duration-per-Occurrence (or Duration-per-Instance): This involves measuring the length of time each individual instance of the target behavior lasts. You would record the duration for each separate episode.
      Example: Tantrum #1 lasted 5 minutes and 12 seconds. Tantrum #2 (later in the session) lasted 12 minutes and 3 seconds.
    • Total Duration: This involves measuring the cumulative (total) amount of time the person engaged in the target behavior across the entire observation period.
      The behavior starts and stops multiple times, you add up the duration of all those instances.
      Example: The total time a student spent actively engaging in on-task academic behavior during a 2-hour session was 1 hour and 17 minutes.
      • When to Use Duration: This measure is most appropriate for behaviors where the length of time the behavior lasts is the primary concern, rather than just how many times it happens.
        It’s useful for behaviors that may occur for extended periods (e.g., on-task behavior, duration of tantrums, time spent engaging in stereotypy, duration of social interaction, time spent engaging in a specific leisure skill).
  • Real-World Examples of Duration Recording:
    • On-Task Behavior (Total Duration):
      • An RBT is observing a student during a 45-minute independent work period.
        The RBT starts a stopwatch each time the student is on-task (as per the operational definition: e.g., eyes on worksheet, pencil moving, not talking to peers) and stops the stopwatch when the student goes off-task.
        The RBT continues to start and stop the stopwatch (or, more efficiently, uses a cumulative timer feature if available) throughout the 45-minute period.
        At the end, the stopwatch displays the total duration of on-task behavior.
      • Example Data: Total on-task duration = 32 minutes out of the 45-minute observation period.
    • Tantrum Behavior (Duration-per-Occurrence):
      • A child has a tantrum.
        The RBT starts a timer when the tantrum begins (e.g., as soon as intense screaming and dropping to the floor starts, per the operational definition) and stops the timer when the tantrum ends (e.g., when the child is calm and quiet for 1 continuous minute, as per the operational definition).
        The RBT records this duration. If another distinct tantrum occurs later in the session, its duration is also recorded separately.
      • Example Data: Tantrum #1: 7 minutes and 15 seconds; Tantrum #2: 4 minutes and 30 seconds.
    • Social Engagement During Play (Total Duration):
      • An RBT is tracking how long a client spends actively engaged in reciprocal (back-and-forth) play with a peer during a 20-minute structured play session.
        The RBT uses a cumulative stopwatch, starting it when reciprocal engagement begins (e.g., both children are interacting with the same toy, taking turns, making eye contact related to the play) and stopping it when engagement ceases (e.g., one child walks away, they start playing independently).
        The RBT then restarts the timer if reciprocal engagement resumes.
      • Example Data: Total social engagement duration = 11 minutes and 20 seconds within the 20-minute session.
  • Key Terms/Vocabulary (Specific to Duration):
    • Onset (of behavior): The precise moment the behavior begins, as defined operationally.
    • Offset (of behavior): The precise moment the behavior ends, as defined operationally.
    • Cumulative Timer: A feature on some stopwatches, timers, or data collection apps that allows the user to stop and start timing repeatedly without resetting the timer to zero each time.
      This makes it easy to accumulate the total time a behavior occurs across multiple instances.
    • Timing Device: Any tool used for measuring time (e.g., stopwatch, clock with a second hand, timer app on a phone or tablet). Accuracy is key.
  • Common Mistakes/Misunderstandings (Specific to Duration Recording):
    • Vague Onset/Offset Definitions in the Operational Definition: If the operational definition doesn’t clearly and objectively state when to start timing and when to stop timing, the duration data collected will be unreliable and inconsistent across observers or even by the same observer over time.
      For example, for “crying,” does timing start with the first tear, the first audible sound, or a specific facial contortion? When does it end when the crying sound stops completely, or after a specified period of quiet?
    • Delay in Starting or Stopping the Timer: Human reaction time can introduce small errors. The RBT must be vigilant in starting and stopping the timing device as close to the true onset and offset of the behavior as humanly possible.
    • Measuring Only One Instance When Total Duration is Needed (or vice-versa): If the goal is to measure the total duration of on-task behavior in a period, the RBT needs to time every instance of on-task behavior and sum them (or use a cumulative timer), not just record the duration of the first or longest instance.
      Conversely, if duration-per-occurrence is needed, each instance must be timed and recorded separately.
    • “Contamination” of Duration by Other Behaviors: For example, if measuring “on-task” duration, and the client briefly asks a relevant clarifying question to the RBT about the task, is that still considered on-task, or is it a momentary off-task behavior that should pause the timer? The operational definition must be precise enough to cover such nuances.
    • Forgetting to Record Units of Time: Recording “5” for duration is meaningless. Is that 5 seconds, 5 minutes, or 5 hours? Always include the unit of time (e.g., sec, min, hr) with the numerical value.

(A-02d) Latency

  • Detailed Definition:
    Latency is the measure of elapsed time between the onset of a specific stimulus (e.g., an instruction, a cue, an environmental event) and the initiation of the subsequent response (the behavior of interest).
    Essentially, it’s how long it takes for someone to start doing something after they’ve been cued or prompted to do it.A shorter latency is often desirable for responses to instructions, social cues, or safety directives.
    A longer latency might be indicative of processing difficulties, hesitation, or non-compliance, and could be a target for intervention.
    • Key Point: Latency measures the time to begin the response, not the time it takes to complete the entire response (that would be duration of the response itself, if measured).
    • When to Use Latency: This measure is important when the speed of responding to a specific stimulus is of clinical interest.
      • Examples: How long it takes a student to start their homework after being told, “Time to do your math”; the time elapsed until a child responds to their name being called; the time taken to initiate a greeting after someone enters the room; the time it takes to comply with a safety instruction.
  • Real-World Examples of Latency Recording:
    • Response to Instruction (Academic Task):
      • An RBT tells a client, “Put your shoes on.” The RBT starts a stopwatch precisely at the end of the verbal instruction (i.e., after the word “on”) and stops the stopwatch the moment the client makes their first physical movement clearly directed towards their shoes (e.g., reaching for a shoe, turning towards the shoe rack).
      • Example Data: Latency to initiate “put shoes on” = 7 seconds.
    • Compliance with a Safety Directive:
      • During a community outing, a client starts to step off the curb into the street without looking. The RBT says firmly and clearly, “Stop!” The RBT measures the time from the end of the word “Stop!” until the client freezes their movement or pulls their foot back onto the curb.
      • Example Data: Latency to comply with “Stop!” = 0.8 seconds.
    • Initiating a Task After an Environmental Cue (Timer):
      • A visual timer signals the end of playtime and the start of cleanup time in a classroom. The RBT measures the latency from the moment the timer buzzes (or lights flash, depending on the timer) until the client picks up the first toy to put away as part of the cleanup routine.
      • Example Data: Latency to begin cleanup after timer cue = 22 seconds.
  • Key Terms/Vocabulary (Specific to Latency):
    • Stimulus Onset: The precise moment the cue, signal, or instruction is presented or completed.
    • Response Initiation/Onset: The precise moment the first part of the target behavior begins. This must be clearly defined.
    • Prompt: A stimulus provided before or during a response to increase the likelihood of the correct response. Latency can sometimes be measured from the presentation of a prompt if that is the specific focus of measurement.
  • Common Mistakes/Misunderstandings (Specific to Latency Recording):
    • Measuring Duration Instead of Latency: A very common error is timing how long the entire response takes from start to finish, rather than just the time from the stimulus until the response starts.
    • Inconsistent Stimulus Delivery: If the instruction or cue (the stimulus) is delivered differently each time (e.g., sometimes loud, sometimes soft, sometimes with an accompanying gesture, sometimes without), it can affect the client’s latency, making the data difficult to interpret and compare.
      The stimulus should be presented as consistently as possible.
    • Starting the Timer Too Early or Too Late: For example, starting the timer while still in the middle of giving the instruction, or waiting until the client has already clearly started moving to initiate the response.
      Precision in starting the timer at the offset of the stimulus is key.
    • Unclear Operational Definition of “Response Initiation”: What exact movement or action constitutes the start of the response? For “put shoes on,” is it when the client looks at their shoes, when they move a hand towards a shoe, or when they actually touch a shoe?
      The operational definition of the response and specific guidance from the supervisor are critical here.
    • Recording “Zero Latency”: While a very, very short latency is theoretically possible, true “zero latency” is exceptionally rare unless the response begins simultaneously with the offset of the stimulus, which is hard to capture.
      Usually, there’s at least a fraction of a second of processing time. However, if a client responds before an instruction is fully completed, this is a different issue to note (e.g., impulsivity or guessing) rather than “zero latency” to the completed instruction.

(A-02e) Interresponse Time (IRT)

  • Detailed Definition:
    Interresponse Time (IRT) measures the amount of time that elapses between two consecutive instances of a specific response class. Essentially, it’s the time from the end (offset) of one instance of the behavior to the beginning (onset) of the next instance of that same (or functionally similar) behavior.
    Shorter IRTs indicate a higher rate of responding for that behavior, while longer IRTs indicate a lower rate. IRT data can be crucial for:
    • Understanding patterns of behavior (e.g., if stereotypy occurs in rapid bursts with short IRTs).
    • Implementing certain differential reinforcement schedules, particularly DRL (Differential Reinforcement of Lower Rates of Behavior), where reinforcement is often delivered if the IRT between responses is above a certain criterion (i.e., the client waits longer between responses).
    • When to Use IRT: This measure is valuable when the timing between successive responses is of clinical interest.
      It’s useful for analyzing the pacing of behavior, identifying rhythmic or stereotypic patterns (which often have very short, consistent IRTs), or for interventions that specifically target the rate of behavior indirectly by reinforcing specific IRT durations.
  • Real-World Examples of IRT Recording:
    • Pacing of Academic Responses on a Worksheet:
      • An RBT is observing a student completing a worksheet of math problems.
        The RBT measures the time from when the student writes the complete answer to problem #1 and lifts their pencil from the paper (this is the end/offset of response 1) until they begin writing the first digit of the answer for problem #2 (this is the beginning/onset of response 2).
        This is repeated for subsequent problems.
      • Example Data: IRT between finishing problem 1 and starting problem 2 = 10 seconds; IRT between finishing problem 2 and starting problem 3 = 15 seconds.
    • Smoking Cessation Program (Increasing Time Between Cigarettes):
      • A client is participating in a program to reduce their smoking.
        Data is collected on the IRT between cigarettes: specifically, the time from when they extinguish one cigarette butt (end of response 1) until they light the next cigarette (beginning of response 2).
        The goal of the intervention would be to gradually increase this IRT.
      • Example Data: IRT between cigarette #1 and cigarette #2 = 2 hours and 15 minutes.
    • Stereotypic Object Tapping:
      • A client repeatedly taps a toy car on the table in a rhythmic fashion.
        The RBT measures the time from the end of one tap (e.g., the car lifts off the table after making contact) to the beginning of the next tap (e.g., the car makes contact with the table again).
        This measurement might reveal very short, consistent IRTs, characteristic of some stereotypic behaviors.
      • Example Data: IRT #1 = 0.5 seconds, IRT #2 = 0.4 seconds, IRT #3 = 0.5 seconds.
  • Key Terms/Vocabulary (Specific to IRT):
    • Consecutive Responses: Two responses of the same type (or belonging to the same response class) occurring one after the other.
    • Response Offset (of the first response): The precise moment one instance of the behavior ends.
    • Response Onset (of the next response): The precise moment the next instance of the same behavior begins.
  • Common Mistakes/Misunderstandings (Specific to IRT Recording):
    • Measuring Duration of Individual Responses Instead of Time Between Them: This is a common point of confusion. IRT is the pause, gap, or interval between two responses, not how long the behaviors themselves last.
    • Measuring IRT Between Two Different Behaviors: IRT is typically measured between instances of the same response class or topographically similar behaviors.
      Measuring the time between a client making a mand (request) and then engaging in a hit is not IRT in the standard, technical sense used for behavior analysis (though it could be a measure of latency to hitting after a mand is denied, for example, which is different).
    • Inconsistent or Vague Definitions of Response Onset and Offset: Just as with duration and latency measurement, if the exact start and end points of the behavior being measured are not clearly and objectively defined in the operational definition, the IRT measurement will be flawed and unreliable.
    • Difficulty Accurately Timing Very Short IRTs with High-Rate Behaviors: If IRTs are extremely short (e.g., fractions of a second, as might occur with rapid vocal tics or some forms of stereotypy), manual timing with a standard stopwatch can be exceedingly difficult and prone to significant error. Specialized timing equipment or video recording and frame-by-frame analysis might be needed for highly accurate measurement of very short IRTs (this would typically be handled or set up by the BCBA).
    • Averaging IRTs Inappropriately During Data Collection: While BCBAs will analyze the distribution and often the average of IRTs, an RBT’s primary role during data collection is to record each individual IRT accurately as it occurs. Simply reporting an “average IRT” without the raw data can mask important variability or patterns in the behavior.

Accurately implementing continuous measurement procedures frequency, rate, duration, latency, and Interresponse Time (IRT) is fundamental to capturing every instance of a target behavior, providing a complete and detailed record.

These tools are invaluable for behaviors with clear onsets and offsets, offering rich data for analysis, but their correct application demands precision and a clear understanding of the common mistakes associated with each.

This detailed examination of A-02 should empower you to execute these five core techniques with confidence and accuracy. For RBTs, mastering the nuances of each continuous method is crucial for providing the high-quality data that informs effective intervention.

Following this, A-03 will shift our focus to Discontinuous Measurement Procedures.

RBT Task List – Measurement (A-01)

RBT Task List – Measurement (A-03)

Leave a Comment